Skip to main content
Log in

The dimensions of stage theories

  • Articles
  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Stage theories are commonly used to represent the structure of cognitive processes in the domains of cognitive processing and cognitive development. Hierarchical unidimensional theories consisting of a single sequence of stages through which development proceeds in one direction only have predominated. However, a review of stage theories revealed that a two-dimensional, interactive (multidimensional) structure is warranted. Particularly in the area of cognitive development, results of research have supported subdivision of the stages of prominent unidimensional theories. Following this trend a small number of theorists have developed multidimensional theories, most of which consist of a sequence of stages in which each stage contains a number of subsidiary stages or levels. The emergence of a similar trend in other domains is examined, and the prospects of using an existing multidimensional framework to enable future comparisons and integration of different stage theories is explored.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, M. J. (1979). Models of word recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 11,133–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Airasian, P. W., & Bart, W. M. (1975). Validating a priori instructional hierarchies. Journal of Educational Measurement, 12,163–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Albus, J. S. (1979). A model of the brain for robot control. Byte, 4(6), 10–34, (7), 54–95, (8), 66–80, (9), 130–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Albus, J. S. (1981). Brains, behavior, and robotics. Peterborough: Byte Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allport, G. W. (1961). Pattern and growth in personality. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston (Originally published in 1937).

    Google Scholar 

  • Alvord, R. W. (1968). Learning and transfer in a concept attainment task: A study of individual differences. Dissertation Abstracts International, 29,473A.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arlin, P. K. (1975). Cognitive development in adulthood: A fifth stage? Developmental Psychology, 11(5), 602–606.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, R. C., & Shriffin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In Spence, K. W., & Spence, J. T. (Eds.). The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol II). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (1979). Level of processing and levels of explanation: Discussion of the papers by Treisman and Bransford, Franks, Morris and Stein. In Cermak, L. S., & Craik, F. I. (Eds.). Levels of processing in human memory. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc. Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1978). The trouble with levels: A reexamination of Craik and Lockhart's framework for memory research. Psychological Review, 85,139–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bart, W. M., & Mertens, D. M. (1979). The hierarchical structure of formal operational tasks. Applied Psychological Measurement, 3(3), 343–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beagles-Roos, J. & Greenfield, P. M. (1979). Development of structure and strategy in two-dimensional pictures. Developmental Psychology, 15(5), 483–494.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beavers, W. R. (1983). Hierarchical issues in a systems approach to illness and health. Family Systems Medicine, 1(1), 47–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biggs, J. B. (1979). Individual differences in study processes and the quality of learning outcomes. Higher Education, 8,381–394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biggs, J. B., & Collis, K. F. (1982). Evaluating the quality of learning. The SOLO taxonomy (Structure of the Observed Learning Outcome). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, B. S., Engelhart, M. D., Furst, F. J., Hill, W. H., & Krathwohl, D. R. (1956). Taxonomy of educational objectives: The classification of educational goals. Handbook 1: Cognitive domain. New York: McKay.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brabeck, M. M. (1984). Longitudinal studies of intellectual development during adulthood: Theoretical and research models. Journal of Research and Development in Education, 17(3), 12–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brainerd, C. J. (1977). Cognitive development and concept learning: An interpretative review. Psychological Bulletin, 94,919–939.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bransford, J. D., Franks, J. J., Morris, C. D., & Stein, B. S. (1979). Some general constraints on research. In Cermak, L. S., & Craik, F. I. (Eds.). Levels of processing in human memory. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc. Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E. (1977). Levels, hierarchies, and the locus of control. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29,181–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bronson, G. (1965). The hierarchical organization of the central nervous system: Implications for learning processes and critical periods in early development. Behavioral Science, 10,7–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1964). The course of cognitive growth. American Psychologist, 19,1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1965). The growth of mind. American Psychologist, 20,1007–1017.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981). The self-attention-induced feedback loop and social facilitation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 7,545–568.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1982). Control theory; A useful conceptual framework for personalitysocial, clinical, and health psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 92(1), 111–135.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Case, R. (1972). Validation of a neo-Piagetian mental capacity construct. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 14,287–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Case, R. (1980). The underlying mechanism of intellectual development. In Kirby, J. R., & Biggs, J. B. (Eds.). Cognition, Development, and Instruction. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Case, R. (1974). Structures and strictures: Some functional limitations on the course of cognitive growth. Cognitive Psychology, 6,544–573.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Case, R. (1985). Intellectual development: Birth to adulthood. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Case, R. (1987). The structure and process of intellectual development. International Journal of Psychology, 22,571–607.

    Google Scholar 

  • Case, R. (1991). Stages in the development of the young child's first sense of self. Developmental Review, 11,210–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Case, R. (1992). The role of the frontal lobes in the regulation of cognitive development. Brain and Cognition, 20,51–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cermak, L. S., & Craik, F. I. (1979). Levels of processing in human memory. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, M. (1988). Contextuality and directionality of cognitive development. Human Development, 31, 92–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, R. A., & Waters, W. F. (1985). Psychophysiological correlates of levels and stages of cognitive processing. Neuropsychologia, 23(2), 243–256.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Collis, A. M., & Quillian, M. R. (1969). Retrieval time from semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8,240–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cometa, M. S., & Eson, M. E. (1978). Logical operations and metaphor interpretation: A Piagetian model. Child Development, 49,649–659.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Commons, M. L., Richards, F. A., & Kuhn, D. (1982). Systematic and metasystematic reasoning: A case for levels of reasoning beyond Piaget's stage of formal operations. Child Development, 53,1058–1069.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11,671–684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Craik, F. I., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and retention of words in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104(3), 268–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davison, M. L., King, P. M., Kitchener, K. S., & Parker, C. A. (1980). The stage sequence concept in cognitive and social development. Developmental Psychology, 16(2), 121–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Mey, M. (1976). The structure of behavior: Heterarchical organisation of actions. Communications and Cognition, 9,269–279.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donders, F. C. (1868/1969). On the speed of mental processes. Acta Psychologica, 30, 412–431. Translated by W. G. Koster from ‘Over de snelheid van psychische processen. Onderzoekingen gedaan in het Physiologisch Laboratorium der Utrechtsche Hoogeschool, 1868-1869, Tweede reeks, II, 92-120.’

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Easley, J. A. (1973). Levels of abstraction and intellectual development. Journal of Children's Mathematical Behavior, 1(2), 76–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Egeth, H., Jonides, J., & Wall, S. (1972). Parallel processing of multielement displays. Cognitive Psychology, 3,674–698.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, H. T. (1980). Some biological bases of cognitive development. Bulletin of The Orton Society, 30, 46–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Estes, W. K. (1972a). An associate basis for coding and organisation in memory. In Melton, A. W., & Martin, E. (Eds.). Coding processes in human memory. Washington, D.C.: V. H. Winston & Sons Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Estes, W. K. (1972b). Interactions of signal and background variables in visual processing. Perception and Psychophysics, 12,278–286.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H. J. (1979). The structure and measurement of intelligence. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fairweather, H., & Hutt, S. J. (1978). On the rate of gain of information in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 26,216–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feigenbaum, E. A., & Feldman, J. (Eds.). (1963). Computers and thought. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, K. W. (1980). A theory of cognitive development: The control and construction of hierarchies of skills. Psychological Review, 87(6), 477–531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, K. W., & Bullock, D. (1984). Cognitive development in school-age children: Conclusions and new directions. In Collins, W. A. (Ed.). Development during middle childhood: The years from six to twelve. Washington, D.C.: National Academy Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, K. W., & Pipp, S. L. (1984). Processes of cognitive development: Optimal level and skill acquisition. In Steinberg, R. I. (Ed.). Mechanisms of cognitive development. New York: W. H. Freeman and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H. (1977). Cognitive development. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, W. L., & Taylor, J. E. (1969). Adaptation of training to individual differences. In Wilson, N. (Ed.). Proceedings of the NA.T.O. conference on ‘manpower research in the defence context’, 1967. London: The English Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frazier, L., & Caldwell, E. (1977). Testing higher cognitive skills in young children. Reading Teacher, 30, 475–478.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (1962). The ego and the id. London: The Hogarth Press. (First published 1927).

    Google Scholar 

  • Fukushima, K. (1984). A hierarchical neural network model for associative memory. Biological Cybernetics, 50,105–113.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Furby, L. (1972). Cumulative learning and cognitive development. Human Development, 15, 265–286.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gagne, R. M. (1965). The conditions of learning. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gagne, R. M., Mayor, J. R., Garstens, H. L., & Paradise, N. E. (1962). Factors in acquiring knowledge of a mathematics task. Psychological monographs, 76(7, No.562).

  • Gagne, R. M. (1968). Contributions of learning to human development. Psychological Review, 75(3), 177–191.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gagne, R. M., & Paradise, N. E. (1961). Abilities and learning sets on knowledge acquisition. Psychological Monographs, 75(14, Whole No. 518).

  • Gibson, K. R. (1981). Comparative neuro-ontogeny: Its implications for the development of human intelligence. In Butterworth, G. (Ed.). Infancy and epistemology: An evaluation of Piaget's theory. Brighton: The Harvester Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glasser, W. (1981). Stations of the mind: New directions for reality therapy. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodson, B. D., & Greenfield, P. M. (1975). The search for structural principles in children's manipulative play: A parallel with linguistic development. Child Development, 46,734–746.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. B. (1972). One second of reading. In Kavanagh, J. F., & Mattingly, I. G. (Eds.). The relationship between speech and reading. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenfield, P. M., Nelson, K., & Saltzman, E. (1972). The development of rulebound strategies for manipulating seriated cups: A parallel between action and grammar. Cognitive Psychology, 3,291–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenfield, P. M., & Schneider, L. (1977). Building a tree structure: The development of hierarchical complexity and interrupted strategies in children's construction activity. Developmental Psychology, 13(4), 299–313

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greeno, J. G., Magone, M. E., & Chaiklin, S. (1979). Theory of constructions and set in problem solving. Memory and Cognition, 7(6), 445–461.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halford, G. S. (1980). A learning set approach to multiple classification: Evidence for a theory of cognitive levels. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 3,409–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halford, G. S. (1982). The development of thought. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc. Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halford, G. S. (1978). Cognitive developmental stages emerging from levels of learning. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 1,341–354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halford, G. S. (1987). A structure-mapping approach to cognitive development. International Journal of Psychology, 22,609–642.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halford, G. S., & Wilson, W. H. (1980). A category theory approach to cognitive development. Cognitive Psychology, 12,356–411.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hansen, J. C., & Hillyard, S. A. (1983). Selective attention to multidimensional auditory stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9(1), 1–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Herriot, P. (1974). Attributes of memory. London: Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1962). Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex. Journal of Physiology, 160, 106–154.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hudspeth, W. J., & Pribram, K. H. (1990). Stages of brain and cognitive maturation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82,881–884.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isaac, D. J., & O'Connor, B. M. (1975). A discontinuity theory of psychological development. Human Relations, 29,41–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, A. R. (1970). Hierarchical theories of mental ability. In Doctrell, W. B. (Ed.). On intelligence. The Toronto symposium on intelligence. London: Methuen & Co. Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kagan, J. (1966). A developmental approach to conceptual growth. In Klausmeier, H. J., & Harris, C. W. (Eds.). Analyses of concept learning. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keats, J. A. (1983). Ability measures and theories of cognitive development. In Wainer, H., Messick, S. (Eds.). Principles of modern psychological measurement. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc. Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keren, G. (1976). Levels of perceptual processing as a function of stimulus material and spatial location. Perception and Psychophysics, 20(1), 37–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitchener, K. S., & King, P. M. (1981). Reflective judgment: Concepts of justification and their relationship to age and education. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 2,89–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitchener, K. S., King, P. M., Wood, P. K., & Davidson, M. L. (1989). Sequentially and consistency in the development of reflective judgment: A six year longitudinal study. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 10,73–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klahr, D., & Wallace, J. G. (1976). Cognitive development: An information-processing view. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc. Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, K., & Saltz, E. (1976). Specifying the mechanisms in a levels-of-processing approach to memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 2(6), 671–679.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koestler, A. (1964). The act of creation. London: Hutchinson & Co. Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence: The cognitive developmental approach to socialisation. In Goslin, D. A. (Ed.). Handbook of socialisation theory and research. Chicago: Rand McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krathwohl, D. R., Bloom, B. S., & Masia, B. B. (1964). Taxonomy of educational objectives: The classifi-cation of educational goals. Handbook II: Affective domain. New York: McKay.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kropp, R. P., Stoker, H. W., & Bashaw, W. L. (1966). The validation of the taxonomy of educational objectives. Journal of Experimental Education, 34,69–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kunen, S., Cohen, R., & Solman, R. (1981). A levels-of-processing analysis of Bloom's taxonomy. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73(2), 202–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • La Berge, D.,&, Samuels,S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 6,293–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LeGare, M. (1980). Overlapping functional systems: A theory for vertebrate central nervous system function in terms of informal systems analysis. Behavioral Science, 25,89–106.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loevinger, J. (1976). Ego development. Washington, D.C.: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loevingez, J., & Knoll, E. (1983). Personality: Stages, traits, and the self. Annual Review of Psychology, 34,195–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. (1961). The role of speech in the regulation of normal and abnormal behavior. London: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mack, W., Lindquist, J. E., & Parham, L. D. (1982). A synthesis of occupational behavior and sensory integration concepts in theory and practice, Part 1. Theoretical foundations. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 36(6), 365–374.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacLean, P. D. (1970). The triune brain, emotion and scientific bias. In Schmitt, F. O. (Ed.). The neurosciences second study program. New York: The Rockefeller University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G. (1968). Association and organisation—facts, fancies and theories. In Dixon, T. R., & Horton, D. L., (Eds.). Verbal behavior and general behavior theory. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G. (1967). Organisation and memory. In Spence, K. W., & Spence, J. T. (Eds.). The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 1). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marken, R. (1982). Intentional and accidental behavior: A control theory analysis. Psychological Reports, 50,647–650.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marken, R. (1980). The cause of control movement in a tracing task. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 51, 755–758.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marmurek, H. H. C. (1977). Processing letters in words at different levels. Memory and Cognition, 5(1), 67–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marmurek, H. H. C., & Briscoe, G. (1982). Orthographic and lexical processing of visual letter strings. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 36(3), 368–387.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maslow, A. H. (1954). Motivation and personality New York: Harper and Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayzner, M. S. (1972). Visual information processing of alphabetic inputs. Psychonomic Monograph Supplements, 4, 239–243.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCall, R. B., Eichorn, D. H., & Hogarty, P. S. (1977). Transitions in early mental development. Monographs for the Society for Research in Child Development, 42,1–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J. L. (1979). On the time relations of mental processes: An examination of systems of processes in cascade. Psychological Review, 86, 287–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88,375–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McLaughlin, G. H. (1963). Psycho-logic: A possible alternative to Piaget's formulation. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 13,61–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merrill, M. D., Barton, K., & Wood L. E. (1970). Specific review in learning a hierarchical imaginary science. Journal of Educational Psychology, 61,102–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior. New York: Henry Holt and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morris, C. D., Bransford, J. D., & Franks, J. J. (1977). Levels of processing versus transfer appropriate processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16,519–533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mowbray, G. H. (1960). Choice reaction time for skilled responses. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12,193–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. (1964). Visual search. Scientific American, 210, (6), 94–102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive Psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A., & Bobrow, D. G. (1975). On data-limited and resource-limited processes. Cognitive Psychology, 7,44–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A., & Bobrow, D. G. (1976). On the role of active memory processes in perception and cognition. In Cofer, C. N. (Ed.). The structure of human memory. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nosal, C. S. (1975). The systems approach to informational processes in the human operator. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 6(4), 180–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J. A. (1970). A mathematical model for the transition rule in Piaget's developmental stages. Acta Psychologica, 32,301–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pew, R. W. (1974). Human perceptual-motor performance. In Kantowitz, B. H. (Ed.) Human information processing: Tutorials in performance and cognition. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc. Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1932). The moral judgment of the child. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1953). The origin of intelligence in the child. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1950). The psychology of intelligence. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Mitchell, R. F. (1967). Chronometric analysis of classification. Psychological Review, 74(5), 392–409.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Postman, L. (1975). Verbal learning and memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 26, 291–335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Powers, W. T. (1973b). Feedback: Beyond behaviorism. Science, 179, 351–356.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Powers, W. T. (1978). Quantitative analysis of purposive systems: Some spadework at the foundations of scientific psychology. Psychological Review, 85, 417–435.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Powers, W. T. (1979). The nature of robots. Byte, 4(6), 132–144, (7), 134–152, (8), 94–116, (9), 96–112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powers, W. T. (1973a). Behavior: The control of perception. London: Wildwood House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powers, W. T. (1980). Systems approach to consciousness. In Davidson, J. M., & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.). The Psychobiology of consciousness. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prater, D. L., & Mayo, N. B. (1984). Cognitive developmental level and syntactic maturity. Journal of Research and Development in Education, 17(3), 1–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reicher, G. M. (1969). Perceptual recognition as a function of meaningfulness of the stimulus material. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 275–280.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ritter, W., Simson, R., Vaughan, H. G., & Macht, M. (1982). Manipulation of event-related potential manifestations of information processing stages. Science, 218,909–910.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, N. (1976). Further verification of Bloom's taxonomy. Journal of Experimental Education, 45(1), 16–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosen, G., & Solman, R. (1984). Recall of key words as a function of Bloom's cognitive level. Educational Psychology, 4(2), 125–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E. (1977). Toward an interactive model of reading. In Dornic, S. (Ed.). Attention and performance (Vol. 1). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc. Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1982). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 2. The contextual enhancement effect and some tests and extensions of the model. Psychological Review, 89,60–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saltz, E. (1971). The cognitive bases of human learning. Homewood, Illinois: Dorsey Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seamon, J. G., & Murray, P. (1976). Depth of processing recall and recognition memory: Differential effects of stimulus meaningfulness and serial position. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 2,680–687.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seddon, G. M. (1978). The properties of Bloom's taxonomy of educational objectives for the cognitive domain. Review of Educational Research, 48,303–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Selman, R. L. (1976). Social cognitive understanding. In Lickona, T. (Ed.). Moral development and behavior. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheppard, J. L. (1974). Concrete operational thought and developmental aspects of solutions to a task based on a mathematical three-group. Developmental Psychology, 10,116–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S. (1976). Three aspects of cognitive development. Cognitive Psychology, 8,481–520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sinclair-de-Zwart, H. (1973). Developmental psycholinguistics. In Elkind, D. & Flavell, J. (Eds.). Studies in cognitive development: Essays in honour of Jean Piaget. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. E. (1968). Choice reaction time: An analysis of the major theoretical positions. Psychological Bulletin, 69,77–110.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. E., & Spoehr, K. T. (1974). Word and letter perception. In Kantowitz, B. H. (Ed.) Human information processing: Tutorials in performance and cognition. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc. Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snarey, J., Kohlberg, L., & Noam, G. (1983). Ego development in perspective: Structural stage, functional phase, and cultural age-period models. Developmental Review, 3,303–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snodgrass, J. G., & Townsend, J. T. (1980). Comparing parallel and serial models: Theory and implementation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 6(2), 330–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanton, W. R. & Keats, J. A. (1986). Intelligence and ordered task complexity. Australian Journal of Psychology, 38,125–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanton, W. R. (1993). A cognitive development framework. Current Psychology: Research and Reviews, 12,26–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stein, B. S. (1978). Depth of processing reexamined: The effects of the precision of encoding and test appropriateness. Journal of Verbal learning and Verbal Behavior, 17,165–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steinberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders' method. Acta Psychologica, 30,276–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thatcher, R. W. (1991). Maturation of the human frontal lobes: Physiological evidence for staging. Developmental Neuropsychology, 7,397–419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Townsend, J. T., & Ashby, F. G. (1983). The stochastic modelling of elementary psychological processes. London: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tryon, W. W. (1993a). Neutral networks: 1. Theoretical unification through connectionism, Clinical Psy-chology Review, 13,341–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tryon, W. W. (1993b). Neutral networks: II. Unified learning theory and behavioral psychotherapy, Clinical Psychology Review, 13,353–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. (1979). Relation between encoding specificity and levels of processing. In Cermak, L. S., & Craik, F. I. (Eds.). Levels of processing in human memory. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc. Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Uzgiris, I. C. (1976). Organisation of sensori-motor intelligence. In Lewis, M. (Ed.). Origins of intelligence. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Essen, D. C. (1979). Visual areas of the mammalian cerebral cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 2,227–263.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. (Translated by E. Hanfmann and G. Vakar). Cambridge, Massachusetts: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walker, L. J., & Taylor, J. H. (1991). Stage transitions in moral reasoning: A longitudinal study of developmental processes. Developmental Psychology, 27,330–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whaley, K. K. (1990). The emergence of social play in infancy: A proposed developmental sequence of infant-adult social play. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 5,347–358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White, R. T. (1973). Research into learning hierarchies. Review of Educational Research, 43,361–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White, S. H. (1965). Evidence for a hierarchical arrangement of learning processes. In Lipsitt, L. P., & Spiker, C. C. (Eds.). Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 2). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, R. T. (1974). The validation of a learning hierarchy. American Educational Research Journal, 11(2), 121–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wickelgren, W. A. (1970). Multitrace strength theory. In Norman, D. A. (Ed.). Models of human memory. New York: Academic Press Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiener, N. (1948). Cybernetics: Or control and communication in the animal and the machine. New York: Wiley Publishing Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilder, J. (1975). The lure of magic thinking. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 29(1), 37–55.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Woodward, W. M. (1983). The development of thinking in young children: The problem of analysis. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 6,441–460.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wortman, P. M., & Greenberg, L. D. (1971). Coding, recoding, and decoding of hierarchical information in long-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10,234–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Stanton, W.R. The dimensions of stage theories. Curr Psychol 21, 176–198 (2002). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-002-1012-0

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-002-1012-0

Keywords

Navigation