Dr. Geary makes a very important point regarding the validity of the
data on the use or non-use of bicycle helmet use abstracted from FARS and
recently published by Cummings, et al, in their June, 2006 paper. This
issue is one of the most important limitations and challenges in the use
of narrative analysis. We previously struggled with a similar issue in a
study published in Injury P...
Dr. Geary makes a very important point regarding the validity of the
data on the use or non-use of bicycle helmet use abstracted from FARS and
recently published by Cummings, et al, in their June, 2006 paper. This
issue is one of the most important limitations and challenges in the use
of narrative analysis. We previously struggled with a similar issue in a
study published in Injury Prevention with regards to PPE use among welders
who had experienced a work-related eye injury [Lombardi et al., 2005]. It
is important to reiterate one of our stated limitations since it appears
relevant to the current discussion.
"The narrative analysis method is also limited by the completeness
and consistency of the available text data [Lincoln et al., 2004].
Additionally, sensitivity is likely to be better than specificity—that is,
when keywords are found in the narrative they probably indicate real
contributions to the incident/injury. It is unknown whether there are
words that were truncated, forgotten, lost in conversation, or abbreviated
by those reporting or recording the claim. Thus, narrative analysis likely
underestimates the magnitude of these contributing factors/circumstances
to eye injuries. (insert helmets)"
As always, however, I find the innovative use of the FARS data by
Cummings, Rivara et al. to provide many excellent examples on the utility
of surveillance and administrative data systems for risk factor
identification and injury prevention.
References
P Cummings, F P Rivara, C M Olson, and K M Smith
Changes in traffic crash mortality rates attributed to use of alcohol, or
lack of a seat belt, air bag, motorcycle helmet, or bicycle helmet, United
States, 1982–2001
Inj Prev 2006; 12: 148-154
Lombardi DA, Pannala R, Sorock GS, et al. Welding related
occupational eye injuries: a narrative analysis. Inj Prev 2005
Jun;11(3):174-9.
Lincoln AE, Sorock GS, Courtney TK, et al. Using narrative text and
coded data to develop hazard scenarios for occupational injury
interventions. Inj Prev 2004;10:249–54.
The research letter from Vardy et al (1) seems to suggest that
getting head
teachers to lecture primary school kids about helmet-wearing made no
difference to helmet-wearing rates among those attending hospital with
cycling injuries. Neither did it alter the proportion of head injuries.
But it
was associated with a reduction in the total number of children attending
hospital with cycling injuries...
The research letter from Vardy et al (1) seems to suggest that
getting head
teachers to lecture primary school kids about helmet-wearing made no
difference to helmet-wearing rates among those attending hospital with
cycling injuries. Neither did it alter the proportion of head injuries.
But it
was associated with a reduction in the total number of children attending
hospital with cycling injuries. The possibility that this might be because
the
lectures deterred kids from cycling (or their parents from allowing them
to do
so) might have occurred to the authors.
The dangerisation of cycling from well meaning attempts to persuade
all
cyclists to wear helmets is one of the biggest barriers to mass cycling
(2,3,4).
Can one really persuade people to take up cycling if one is pushing the
message that cycling is so dangerous a helmet should always be worn? In
countries with mass cycling such as The Netherlands and Denmark, very few
wear helmets. The danger for cyclists seems to fall with more cycling,
not
more helmet wearing (5).
In Britain cycling remains no more dangerous that other pastimes and
modes
of transport. Cycling is safer per km traveled than walking (6).
Measured in
terms of time spent cycling is about as risky as being a car occupant (7).
The
authors point out that 34% of cyclists requiring hospital admission have
head
injuries but they make no effort to put this in context. In 2002-3 the
figure
for pedestrians nationally was 43.7% (8). Quite why cyclists should be
singled
out for the helmet treatment is hard to understand, but the case for
helmets
rests on portraying cycling as dangerous. If one is being consistent the
same
concern should be extended to pedestrians and car occupants too, where the
potential exists to save lots more from brain injury. 86% of pedestrians
and
motor vehicle occupants who die in crashes suffer lethal head injuries
(9).
The figure for cyclists is 82%. The authors could consider asking all head injured road users about helmet use. They could campaign for all road
users to wear helmets of some sort.
The Wishaw and Glasgow Royal Infirmary A+E departments see very few
cycle
related head injuries. They will see a large amount of medical
emergencies
related to cardiovascular disease. Cyclists tend to live longer and
suffer less
cardiovascular disease than other people (10,11,12). Whether helmeted or
not it will be good for the local kids health to get into the habit of
cycling.
Let’s not discourage them by pretending it is unusually dangerous.
Today’s
obese, inactive, car bound kids; too scared to cycle are tomorrow’s
cardiovascular cripples.
References
1 Vardy et al, Injury Prevention 2006; 12; 271-272.
8 Parliamentary Question by Brian Jenkins MP, answered by Dr Ladyman,
Dept of Health. Hansard, Written Answers, page 17-8W, 10th November
2003.
9 The pattern of injury in fatal cycle accidents and the possible
benefits of
cycle helmets, Kennedy, British Journal of Sports Medicine, 1996 vol30
p130-133.
10 All-cause mortality associated with physical activity during
leisure time,
work, sports, and cycling to work, Arch Intern Med. 2000; 160:1621-1628.
11 Cycling towards Health and Safety, BMA.
12 Transport and Health, Dr H Rutter for Oxfordshire Health
Authority,
2000.
In an
editorial [1], you presented data from a study of peer reviews of 20
randomly selected papers submitted to Injury Prevention. Each paper was
independently reviewed by three reviewers, who score...
In an
editorial [1], you presented data from a study of peer reviews of 20
randomly selected papers submitted to Injury Prevention. Each paper was
independently reviewed by three reviewers, who scored the paper as low, medium,
or high on four dimensions: significance, appropriateness, science, and
writing. The editorial concluded that agreement among the reviewers was good.
In fact, according to the data presented, agreement on some dimensions was
barely better than would be expected by chance, and on only one dimension was
agreement significantly better than would be expected by chance in a sample of
20 papers.
The
expected agreement score among three reviewers, assuming random assignment of
scores, can be calculated analytically. In your analysis, you defined the
agreement score as 100% if all three reviewers agreed, 66% if two reviewers
agreed, and 0% if all three gave different scores. The mathematical expected
value of the agreement score is 100%×P(all 3 reviewers agree)+66%×P(2 agree)+0%×P(none agree),
where P(-) denotes the probability of an event. Under random assignment,
the probability that 3 reviewers agree is 1/9, that 2 agree is 6/9, and that
none agree is 2/9 (see appendix). Therefore, the expected agreement score would
be 100%×1/9+66%×6/9+0%×2/9 = 55.1%. The reported agreement scores for the significance (56%),
science (60%), and writing (62%) dimensions are barely better than this.
This
problem also lends itself well to a computer simulation. I wrote a program to
randomly generate scores on a three-point scale for each of 3 reviewers. The
program calculated an agreement score as described above. This was repeated for
20 papers, and the mean agreement score was calculated. Then this was repeated
10,000 times to evaluate the distribution of mean agreement scores in samples
of size 20. The computer simulation does not require any probabilistic
calculations and is independent of the analytical approach described above. The
simulation results were: mean score: 55.1%; SE: 7.0%; 2.5th and 97.5th
percentiles (i.e. an interval which contains 95% of the means from the
simulated distribution): (41.3%, 67.9%). Only the agreement score you reported
for appropriateness (69%) was outside this interval.
These
results do not necessarily suggest that all three reviewers perform no better
than chance. If each paper has a particular score which is correct, and one
reviewer always scores each paper correctly, while the
other two reviewers score papers randomly, the agreement among the three
reviewers would be the same as if all three reviewers scored papers randomly.
However, this makes a large difference in the agreement between the reviewers
and the correct score. If all three reviewers score randomly, the probability
that at least two will agree with the correct score is only 7/27, and the
probability that at least two will agree on an incorrect score is 14/27.
If one reviewer scores correctly, and the other two are random, the probability
that at least two will agree with the correct score is 5/9, and the probability
that two will agree on an incorrect score is 2/9.
If
two reviewers always score papers correctly, and the third scores randomly, the
expected agreement score would be 77.3%.
Your
study suggests that agreement among Injury Prevention peer reviewers may
be little better than random. As a reader, I find this discouraging. As a
sometime author (not in Injury Prevention), I am sorry to say that I do
not find it surprising.
Appendix
There
are many ways to calculate the probability that 3, 2, or 0 reviewers agree with
each other under random assignment. The method that makes this calculation
easiest to understand is to enumerate all the possible ways that a paper can be
scored (hopefully, this will resolve the disagreement between experts mentioned
in the editorial). Since each of 3 reviewers can assign any one of 3 scores,
there are 3×3×3 = 27 different ways to score each paper. Say we
denote the scores as 1, 2, or 3, and that we denote the 3 reviewers’ scores for
each paper as a triplet in which the scores for reviewers A, B, and C are in
the first, second, and third positions respectively. The 3 combinations in
which all 3 reviewers agree are 111, 222, and 333. The 18 combinations in which
2 agree are 112, 113, 121, 131, 122, 133, 221, 223, 212, 232, 211, 233, 331,
332, 313, 323, 311, and 322. The 6 combinations in which none agree are 123,
132, 213, 231, 312, and 321. Therefore the probability that all 3 reviewers agree
is 3/27 = 1/9, that 2 agree is 18/27 = 6/9, and that none agree is 6/27 = 2/9.
Note that the probabilities sum to 1, as they must.
References
[1]Pless IB. When
reviewers disagree.InjPrev
2006;12:211.
Cummings et al. assume that bike helmets prevent 65% of deaths.[1] Yet a study of cyclist crashes in Brisbane concluded that helmets would prevent very few fatalities. All deaths were caused by bike/motor vehicle collisions. For 13 of the 14 non-helmeted cyclists who died, there was no indication that a helmet would have made any difference. The authors were very concerned about brain da...
Cummings et al. assume that bike helmets prevent 65% of deaths.[1] Yet a study of cyclist crashes in Brisbane concluded that helmets would prevent very few fatalities. All deaths were caused by bike/motor vehicle collisions. For 13 of the 14 non-helmeted cyclists who died, there was no indication that a helmet would have made any difference. The authors were very concerned about brain damage from rotational injuries and recommended developing a test to measure sliding impact friction of helmets.[2]
Cyclist deaths were also investigated in Auckland. 16 of 19 non-helmeted cyclists died from multiple injuries, so helmets would not have changed the outcome. Only one cyclist died of head injuries in a bike-only crash, the most likely situation for a helmet to help. That cyclist died despite wearing a helmet. The authors concluded: "This study indicates that the compulsory wearing of suitable safety helmets by cyclists is unlikely to lead to a great reduction in fatal injuries, despite their enthusiastic advocacy."[3]
In the three years after helmets were made compulsory in New South Wales, Australia, 80% of fatally injured cyclists wore helmets, an almost identical proportion to population wearing rates (75% of children, 84% of adults), again suggesting that helmets are ineffective at preventing fatalities.[4]
Comparing Australia-wide fatalities in 1988 (before any helmet law) with 1994 (when all states had enforced laws and about 80% helmet wearing); cyclist, pedestrian and all road user deaths fell by 35%, 36% and 38% respectively; head-injury deaths fell by 30%, 38% and 42%. Thus the reductions for cyclists were less than for other road users. Factoring in the reduction in cycling, cyclists were probably at greater risk with compulsory helmet laws than without them.[5]
In inner London, 58% of cyclist fatalities were caused by collisions with heavy goods vehicles, as were 30% of those in outer London.[6] The idea that a polystyrene helmet could be of significant benefit in such circumstances borders on the absurd. The well-known tragic case of 4 helmeted cyclists killed by a car travelling at 50 miles/hr demonstrates that cyclists often die in impacts too severe for a helmet to help.[7]
Riley Geary explained that helmet-wearing status in the FARS database grossly underestimates the true value – many state agencies do not have a check-box for helmet use on their forms and unknowns seem to have been incorrectly recorded as non-wearers.[8] The claims of Cummings et al.[1], based on incorrect helmet wearing rates, and an assumption of the ability of helmets to prevent mortality that bears no relationship whatsoever to information from fatality data, might be dismissed as "enthusiastic advocacy", a classic case of GIGO (garbage in, garbage out).
But in a world of limited resources, there is a sinister side to unrealistic and exaggerated claims – they divert funding away from measures that really could save lives. The only cycling fatality of which I have personal knowledge happened where an off-road cycleway intersects a minor road. It was a difficult crossing; cyclists had to ride carefully through a line of parked cars. By the time cross-traffic was visible, cyclists were almost in front of it, a problem that had been drawn to the attention of the local council. Despite his helmet, a teenager died of head injury after an emergency operation failed to stop the swelling in his skull.
The evidence cited above indicates that forcing cyclists to wear helmets saves very few lives. Other measures, such as guidelines to prevent car parking in places where it obscures sightlines, exploring ways of reducing the disproportionate numbers of circulating cyclists hit by motorists entering roundabouts,[9] random breath testing, speed cameras, and fixing up accident blackspots[10] could save many more.
Perhaps the authors of this article would like to estimate how many more lives might be saved if the considerable efforts currently spent exhorting cyclists to wear helmets were instead spent on making the roads safer for cyclists?
References
1. Cummings P, Rivara FP, Olson CM, Smith KM. Changes in traffic crash mortality rates attributed to use of alcohol, or lack of a seat belt, air bag, motorcycle helmet, or bicycle helmet, United States, 1982-2001. Inj Prev 2006;12(3):148-154.
2. Corner JP, Whitney CW, O'Rourke N, Morgan DE. Motorcycle and bicycle protective helmets: requirements resulting from a post crash study and experimental research. Federal Office of Road Safety, Report CR 55., 1987.
3. Sage M, Cairns F, Koelmeyer T, Smeeton W. Fatal injuries to bicycle riders in Auckland. N Z Med J. 1985;98:1073-4.
4. Robinson DL. Head injuries and bicycle helmet laws. Accid Anal Prevent 1996;28:463-475.
5. Curnow WJ. The Cochrane collaboration and bicycle helmets. Acc Anal Prevent 2005;37(3):569-73.
6. Gilbert K, McCarthy M. Deaths of cyclists in London 1985-92: the hazards of road traffic. BMJ 1994;308:1534-1537.
It was not only fashionable but life saving for motorcyclists to use
helmet while transiting in the 70s and 80s.Limited best practise based on
knowledge couple with deteriorating standard of education and not only
cost and warm climate may be the attributing factors resulting in the
decline of helmet use in the present Nigeria.
Also, cultural or perhaps religious reasons may explain the use...
It was not only fashionable but life saving for motorcyclists to use
helmet while transiting in the 70s and 80s.Limited best practise based on
knowledge couple with deteriorating standard of education and not only
cost and warm climate may be the attributing factors resulting in the
decline of helmet use in the present Nigeria.
Also, cultural or perhaps religious reasons may explain the use of
helmet: a typical Nigerian Northerner may decline helmet use since; it
will compromise his wearing of the ‘Ulla’- traditional hat. The later is
made of cotton fabric that cannot withstand impact on the road should
there be collision. Elsewhere, the use of helmet is seen as a colonial
imposition that should be resisted similar to the phobia for seat belts
use in commercial vehicles. This Author was queried on several occasions
by commercial vehicle Drivers, if he was a Caucasian to insist on the seat
belt use.
Enacting laws and enforcing same with policing by Road Safety
Commission is wishful thinking since, it is not in our nature to respect
laws whose benefits may not be immediately appreciated.
The best option to a sustainable helmet use in Nigeria anchors on
sound health promotion advocacy which should compliment the knowledge of
injuries; morbidity and mortality related to Motorcyclist/Bicyclist who
did not use helmet while on motion. The relative cost of helmet oppose to
hospitalization cost and probably the risk of disability and death.
I am sure with time, People's altitudes will change and it will be
natural to adopt invaluable safety practises with the hope of preventing
injuries caused by not using helmet. Evaluation of such a process will be
desirable.
In response to our article, Burdett makes two main criticisms. The
first relates to the issue of level of cycling activity in the community
post-legislation. The second relates to our interpretation of the evidence
for child cyclist helmet wearing when accompanying adults are helmeted
compared with non-helmeted children. We consider these points separately.
In response to our article, Burdett makes two main criticisms. The
first relates to the issue of level of cycling activity in the community
post-legislation. The second relates to our interpretation of the evidence
for child cyclist helmet wearing when accompanying adults are helmeted
compared with non-helmeted children. We consider these points separately.
On the issue of the level of cycling post-legislation, we commented
on this in the text. The analysis of this complex issue cannot be solved
by the rudimentary analysis provided by Burdett. For example, cycling
activity conclusions require a larger overall sample, conducted over many
more sampling sites, accounting for weather and road construction
differences, and changing demographics within a region. In Alberta, large
population influxes have changed the dynamics of both Calgary and Edmonton
since the original survey. Despite these issues and the variations in
methodology between the 2000 and 2004 surveys, Burdett concludes that “The
proportion of cyclists affected by the law dropped from over 25% of those
observed in 2000 to approximately 15% of those observed at the same sites
in 2004.” Burdett goes on to call this a “disturbing indicator” that
should have caught our attention. Clearly, this should not have caught our
attention, since the analysis performed by Burdett cannot reasonably reach
the conclusion that helmet laws are somehow responsible for decreased
cycling activity. A control for time trends in the demographic
distribution of the population studied would have been, at the very least,
an essential element of any comprehensive assessment. The aim of our
report was to examine the prevalence of helmet use and comment on the
implications of changes in this outcome. A much more comprehensive and
considered assessment of changes in levels of cycling would be required to
evaluate the helmet legislation’s influence on cycling. Moreover, drawing
the conclusion that this is somehow partly responsible for the rising
trend of childhood obesity is, quite frankly, fear mongering.
On the issue of how much more likely children are to be wearing a
helmet if accompanied by a helmeted adult vs. a non-helmeted child
companion, Burdett argues that the relevant comparison is with non-helmeted adult companions. Our intention was to illustrate the degree of
variation in helmet use depending on the helmet and age characteristics of
companions. We do, however, agree that the comparison Burdett suggests is
informative. Certainly an almost 2.5 fold increase in the likelihood of
helmet use when a child is accompanied by a helmeted compared with a non-helmeted adult in the study by Khambalia et al is noteworthy.(1) Referring
back to the original report by Nykolyshyn et al,(2) which presents the
baseline data for the present study, the results also suggest that a
significantly greater proportion of children wear helmets when accompanied
by a helmet wearing adult, compared with a non-helmeted adult. We thank
Burdett for this comment and the opportunity to point out the additional
evidence that suggests if adults wear helmets, accompanying children are
more likely to as well.
The issue of reduced cycling raised by Robinson has been addressed in
our response to Burdett. In addition, this issue and the other issues she
raises about consideration of head injury trends and the evidence on
helmet effectiveness have been previously addressed elsewhere.(3-5) Most
of the evidence cited by Robinson is not systematically collected and
suffers from selection bias. Clearly, Robinson has focused on the fatality
end of the injury spectrum and argues that helmets fail to protect riders
because of the large kinetic energy of traumatic events, especially those
involving motor vehicles. Despite this, well-accepted systematic reviews
arrive at different conclusions. We suggest the readers draw their own
conclusions. Finally, we take exception to the comment that “the
significant reduction in children's cycling relative to adults is enough
to suggest that the law should be repealed.” We firmly believe that
decisions regarding helmet legislation should be based upon the best
available evidence, a balanced assessment of the risks and harms, and
avoidance of personal opinion and rhetoric.
Respectfully,
Brent Hagel,
Brian Rowe
REFERENCES
1. Khambalia A, Macarthur C, Parkin PC. Peer and adult companion
helmet use is associated with bicycle helmet use by children. Pediatrics
2005;116:939-42.
2. Nykolyshyn K, Petruk J, Wiebe N, Cheung M, Belton K, Rowe BH. The
use of bicycle helmets in a western Canadian province without legislation.
Canadian Journal of Public Health 2003;94:144-148.
3. Cummings P, Rivara FP, Thompson DC, Thompson RS. Misconceptions
regarding case-control studies of bicycle helmets and head injury.
Accident Analysis & Prevention 2006;38:636-643.
4. Hagel B, Macpherson A, Rivara FP, Pless B. Arguments against
helmet legislation are flawed. British Medical Journal 2006;332:725-726.
5. Hagel BE, Pless IB. A critical examination of arguments against
bicycle helmet use and legislation. Accident Analysis & Prevention
2006;38:277-278.
In their report on bicycle helmet use[1], Hagel et al recommend that Alberta's child helmet law be extended to include adults. They base this on (a) an increase in the rate of helmet use among the age group affected (under 18 years of age) from two years before the introduction of helmet legislation in 2002 to two years after, and (b) children being observed riding at higher rates of helmet use when accompa...
In their report on bicycle helmet use[1], Hagel et al recommend that Alberta's child helmet law be extended to include adults. They base this on (a) an increase in the rate of helmet use among the age group affected (under 18 years of age) from two years before the introduction of helmet legislation in 2002 to two years after, and (b) children being observed riding at higher rates of helmet use when accompanied by helmeted adults.
Aside from the utility of helmets, important in any consideration of helmet legislation is the impact helmet laws may have on the level of cycling. In jurisdictions where helmet laws have been enforced, cycling has declined[2]. Although cycling may not decline in jurisdictions where laws are not enforced, immediate increases in post-law helmet use rates are unlikely to be maintained. In the Borough of East York, Toronto, Ontario's child helmet law was not enforced. There the helmet use rate among children initially increased[3] but four years later it fell to pre-law levels[4]. In respect of Alberta, Hagel et al said they could not comment on the number of cyclists seen in the 2000 and 2004 observations. That was unfortunate since a repetition in 2004 of the year 2000 count design should have been relatively straightfoward. Regardless, an analysis
of the data they did collect suggests that a post-law decline in child cycling occurred. The proportion of cyclists affected by the law dropped from over 25% of those observed in 2000 to approximatly 15% of those observed at the same sites in 2004. This is a disturbing indicator which ought to have garnered the attention of the authors. Any decline in a healthy and life-extending activity like cycling should be of serious concern to policy makers in view of the increasing trend in child obesity in Canada and elsewhere.
To further support their recommendation, data from Toronto collected principally in the 1990's of child cyclists under 15 years of age is cited[5]. They say "considering that children riding with helmeted adults
are almost 10 times more likely to be wearing a helmet than children riding with nonhelmeted child companions, policy makers should consider extending current children-only helmet legislation in Alberta and other locations". There is a problem with this logic. In considering such an
extension, the relevant helmet use comparison is not with non-helmeted child companions, after all they are already subject to Alberta's law. The correct comparison is with non-helmeted adult companions. While these data were available in the cited Toronto report and produce a figure of just under 2.5 times more likely, more recent and pertinent companion data were available to the authors from the results of the year 2000 observations in Alberta[6]. These show that the helmet wearing rate of children riding with non-helmeted adults was 84%, whereas when riding with helmeted adults it was 99%. Thus children were only 0.15 times less likely to wear helmets when riding with non-helmeted adults. Given the significant difference of this likelihood compared to the stated Toronto "10 times more likely" using a different age group, a serious oversight or an error in judgement in omitting this fact has occurred.
When it comes to issues concerning public policy, it is essential that those who are in a position to influence policy-makers do not ignore obvious indicators or omit relevant data that could have a bearing on
subsequent decisions.
Avery Burdett
References
1.Hagel BE, Rizkallah JW, Lamy A, Belton KL, Jhangri GS, Cherry N,et al. Bicycle helmet prevalence two years after the introduction of mandatory use legislation for under 18 year olds in Alberta, Canada.
Inj Prev 2006;12(4):262-265.
2. Robinson DL. No clear evidence from countries that have enforced the wearing of helmets. BMJ 2006;332:722-725.
3. Macpherson AK, Parkin PC and To TM. Mandatory helmet legislation and children's exposure to cycling. Injury Prevention 2001;7:228-230
4. Macpherson AK, Macarthur C, To TM, Chipman ML, Wright JG and Parkin PC. Economic disparity in bicycle helmet use by children six years after the introduction of legislation. Injury Prevention 2006;12:231-235.
5. Khambalia A, Macarthur C, Parkin PC. Peer and adult companion helmet use is associated with bicycle helmet use by children. Pediatrics 2005;116:939–42.
6. Alberta Centre for Injury Control and Research. Bicycle Helmets Observational Study Summary.
http://www.med.ualberta.ca/acicr/download/bikesumm.pdf [accessed August 8,
2006]
There is considerable debate about enforced helmet laws. Surveys in Australia counted several thousand cyclists before and after legislation, at the same sites, observation times and time of year. Percent helmet
wearing (%HW) increased mainly because non-helmeted cyclists were discouraged from cycling – reductions in numbers counted were 2 to 15 times greater than the increases in numbers wearing helme...
There is considerable debate about enforced helmet laws. Surveys in Australia counted several thousand cyclists before and after legislation, at the same sites, observation times and time of year. Percent helmet
wearing (%HW) increased mainly because non-helmeted cyclists were discouraged from cycling – reductions in numbers counted were 2 to 15 times greater than the increases in numbers wearing helmets.[1] Despite
the large increases in %HW, there was no obvious response in percent head injury, suggesting that helmet laws have very little benefit to counteract the drawback of discouraging this healthy exercise and environmentally-friendly transport.[2]
It is therefore surprising that Hagel et al. consider only %HW and try to draw useful conclusions from an outcome that everyone expected – all studies show enforced laws increase %HW.
Despite limitations, Alberta’s data strongly suggest that cycling was discouraged. Child cyclists fell from 10.3% to 4.8% of all cyclists, and teenagers from 15.4 to 10.3%, a very significant reduction in the proportions of children and adolescents (p=0.0015). Relative to numbers
of adults counted, the data suggest that about 50% of children and a third of teenagers were discouraged from cycling.
Post-law, remarkably few children were counted – only 13 primary school children and 28 adolescents – at all 22 observation sites. With 3 observers per site, that equates to 0.2 children and 0.4 adolescents per observer per site. Even though pedestrians were also recorded, surely the
survey could have been designed to at least count all cyclists? It would be unfortunate if this were the only information on cycle-use before and after Alberta’s helmet law. But if so, the significant reduction in children’s cycling relative to adults is enough to suggest that the law should be repealed.
Indeed, before helmet laws can be recommended, there should be
evidence of substantial reductions in head injuries that clearly outweigh
losses from discouraging cycling. Unfortunately, the authors provide no
evidence of this. Preliminary reports stated that head injuries doubled
from 5% to more than 10% of cyclist injuries.[3] Although this may have
been due partly to changes in data coding, the increase in %HI was
slightly greater for children than adults,[3] the opposite of what would
be expected if helmets are beneficial, given that %HW of children
increased, but %HW of adults did not.
Two other issues deserve comment. First, the authors state:
“Considering that children riding with helmeted adults are almost 10 times
more likely to be wearing a helmet than children riding with non-helmeted
child companions, policy makers should consider extending current children-only helmet legislation in Alberta and other locations.” In fact, the
pre-law survey in Alberta shows that 81% of children riding with non-helmeted adults wore helmets,[4] so in reality forcing adults to wear a
helmet is unlikely to make much difference.
Second the authors claim that two systematic reviews found that
helmets reduce fatal injuries by 73%. Only one review is cited, and that
simply lists a crude odds ratio, based on 47 fatalities, with no
adjustment for confounders.[5] Helmet wearers are more likely to obey
traffic laws,[6] wear fluorescent clothing and use lights at night[7] and
ride in playgrounds or bicycle paths than city streets.[8] All these
factors affect the risk of bike/motor vehicle collisions, the main cause
of cyclist fatalities. There is no way of knowing whether helmet wearers
had fewer deaths relative to the number of minor injuries (but not
necessarily population wearing rates) because of these factors, or because
of helmets.
Recent research showed that, even with adjustment for confounders,
case-control studies can produce incorrect and misleading results. For
example, a systematic review of more than 30 studies of hormone
replacement therapy (HRT) concluded that it reduced the risk of heart
disease by 50%. We now know that HRT does not decrease and probably
increases the risk of heart disease.[9] To avoid such problems,
researchers should cite only reliable evidence, and discount odds ratios
with no attempt to adjust for counfounders.
One reliable source of evidence is a detailed study of serious head
injuries to cyclists in Brisbane. Based on the type and severity of
injuries, it concluded that helmets would prevent very few fatalities. All
deaths were caused by bike/motor vehicle collisions. For 13 of the 14 non-helmeted cyclists who died, there was no indication that a helmet would
have made any difference. The authors were very concerned about brain
damage from rotational injuries and recommended developing a test to
measure sliding impact friction of helmets.[10]
Cyclist deaths were also investigated in Auckland. 16 of 19 non-helmeted cyclists died from multiple injuries, so helmets would not have
changed the outcome. Only one cyclist died of head injuries in a bike-only
crash, the most likely situation for a helmet to help. That cyclist died
despite wearing a helmet. The authors concluded: "This study indicates
that the compulsory wearing of suitable safety helmets by cyclists is
unlikely to lead to a great reduction in fatal injuries, despite their
enthusiastic advocacy."[11]
In the three years after helmets were made compulsory in New South
Wales, Australia, 80% of fatally injured cyclists wore helmets, an almost
identical proportion to population wearing rates (75% of children, 84% of
adults), again suggesting that helmets are ineffective at preventing
fatalities.[1]
Comparing Australia-wide fatalities in 1988 (before any helmet law)
with 1994 (when all states had enforced laws and about 80% helmet
wearing); cyclist, pedestrian and all road user deaths fell by 35%, 36%
and 38% respectively; head-injury deaths fell by 30%, 38% and 42%. Thus
the reductions for cyclists were less than for other road users. Factoring
in the reduction in cycling, cyclists were probably at greater risk with
compulsory helmet laws than without them.[12]
In inner London, 58% of cyclist fatalities were caused by collisions
with heavy goods vehicles, as were 30% of those in outer London. It is
implausible that a polystyrene helmet could be of significant benefit in
such circumstances. The well-known tragic case of 4 helmeted cyclists
killed by a car travelling at 50 miles/hr demonstrates that cyclists often
die in impacts too severe for a helmet to help.[13]
Thus the most reliable evidence suggests that helmets prevent few, if
any, fatalities. In Australia, implementation of other measures, such as
random breath testing, speed cameras, and fixing up accident blackspots,
resulted in large and immediate reductions in fatalities.[1]
In conclusion, the survey data for Alberta show a significant
reduction in the proportions of children and teenagers, strongly
suggesting the main effect of the law was to discourage cycling. Unless
the authors can demonstrate a large response in percent head injury
coinciding with the change in %HW that clearly outweighs the lost health
and environmental benefits from reduced cycling, perhaps in future
attention can be focussed on measures (such as those listed above) that
have been shown to reduce injuries, instead of reducing cycling.
Dr Dorothy L Robinson
References
1 Robinson DL. Head injuries and bicycle helmet laws. Accid Anal
Prevent 1996;28:463-475.
2 Robinson DL. No clear evidence from countries that have enforced
the wearing of helmets. BMJ 2006;332:722-725.
3 Sands D. Helmet law stats called surprise. Calgary Sun 2003
Saturday, July 12, 2003.
4 Alberta Centre for Injury Control & Research. Bicycle
Observational Study Summary. 2001.
5 Attewell R, Glase K, McFadden M. Bicycle helmet efficacy: a meta-
analysis. Accid Anal Prev 2001;33:345–52.
6 Farris C, Spaite DW, Criss EA, Valenzuela TD, Meislin HW.
Observational evaluation of compliance with traffic regulations among
helmeted and nonhelmeted bicyclists. Ann Emerg Med 1997;29(5):625-9.
7 McGuire L, Smith N. Cycling safety: injury prevention in Oxford
cyclists. Inj Prevent 2000;6(4):285-7.
8 DiGuisseppi CG, Rivara FP, Koepsell TD. Bicycle helmet use by
children. Evaluation of a community-wide helmet campaign. JAMA
1989;262:2256-61.
9 Petitti D. Commentary: hormone replacement therapy and coronary
heart disease: four lessons. Int J Epidemiol 2004;33(3):461-3.
10 Corner JP, Whitney CW, O'Rourke N, Morgan DE. Motorcycle and
bicycle protective helmets: requirements resulting from a post crash study
and experimental research. Federal Office of Road Safety, Report CR 55.,
1987.
11 Sage M, Cairns F, Koelmeyer T, Smeeton W. Fatal injuries to
bicycle riders in Auckland. N Z Med J. 1985;98:1073-4.
12 Curnow WJ. The Cochrane collaboration and bicycle helmets. Acc
Anal Prevent 2005;37(3):569-73.
13 Gilbert K, McCarthy M. Deaths of cyclists in London 1985-92: the
hazards of road traffic. BMJ 1994;308(6943):1534-7.
McEvoy et al (2006) provide empirical evidence to support the case
that distractions for the driver are an important feature of road crashes.
There should be nothing too surprising in this; after all, many
authorities recognise that an enforcible code of behaviour must be applied
to public-service drivers; bus passengers are not likely to feel at ease
with a driver whose attention deviates from the task in hand.
McEvoy et al (2006) provide empirical evidence to support the case
that distractions for the driver are an important feature of road crashes.
There should be nothing too surprising in this; after all, many
authorities recognise that an enforcible code of behaviour must be applied
to public-service drivers; bus passengers are not likely to feel at ease
with a driver whose attention deviates from the task in hand.
The real problem is of course the private automobile driver, always a
difficult creature to control. No doubt, education would help. However,
this is an uphill task: for example, I imagine few would suggest that
restrictions on mobile-phone use in jurisdictions such as the UK have been
notably successful, even when backed up by punishment (1).
Nonetheless, one practice that seems unnecessarily gratuitous is the
portrayal of driving on TV: frequently, interviews or monologues to the
camera are undertaken while the speaker is driving, perhaps in fast-moving
and heavy traffic. Often, this adds absolutely nothing to the content of
the programme; for example, I can think of a recent monologue by the
presenter of a BBC TV programme about sites of battles that predate mass
car usage by many years!
In the UK, there has been much comment about driving-related
programmes - BBC's "Top Gear" is a prime example - which seem to promote a
cavalier attitude to road safety. I argue that the problem is more
insidious. Policies to control driver distractions might be given greater
legitimacy in the eyes of the driving public, if a strict code of conduct
could be imposed on the media, with the abolishment of programme
presentation while driving as a priority.
References
(1) Walker L, Williams J, Janrozik K. Unsafe driving behaviour and
four wheel drive vehicles: observational study. BMJ 2007 333:71
According to good public policy, all laws with potentially
detrimental effects (such as reduced cycling and reduced safety in
numbers) should be evaluated. Far from being selective, my review
examined every jurisdiction with a large increase in helmet wearing (more
than 40 percentage points within a year). If helmet laws were beneficial,
there should have been an obvious response. Yet there was no c...
According to good public policy, all laws with potentially
detrimental effects (such as reduced cycling and reduced safety in
numbers) should be evaluated. Far from being selective, my review
examined every jurisdiction with a large increase in helmet wearing (more
than 40 percentage points within a year). If helmet laws were beneficial,
there should have been an obvious response. Yet there was no clear
benefit to offset the expected harm from reduced cycling.[1]
Decisions concerning helmet legislation should, as Hagel and Rowe
suggest, be based on the best available evidence. It would be naïve to
assume that, even if voluntary wearing were beneficial, the same would be
true for helmet laws. Case-control studies cannot determine the effects
of reduced cycling, reduced safety in number or risk compensation.
Consequently, the only reliable way to determine if helmet laws are
beneficial is evaluate their effects on cycle use and head injury rates,
in comparison to the cost buying millions of helmets or introducing other
road safety measures.
It is surprising and disappointing that Hagel et al. chose not to
collect the information needed to evaluate Alberta’s helmet law. Instead,
they devoted 3.5 pages of Injury Prevention to the fact that, at 22 sites
in Edmonton, helmet use of adults remained stable. In contrast, at these
sites, in 2000, 29 children and 17 adolescents wore helmets, 37 children
and 81 adolescents did not. In 2004, 13 children and 21 adolescents wore
helmets; 0 children and 7 adolescents did not. Although the study was not
designed to measure cycle use, this undeniably represents a significant
reduction in the proportion of children and adolescents, compared to
adults who were not affected by the legislation.
The onus should be on those who advocate laws taking away freedom to
choose to prove those laws are beneficial. My systematic review found no
evidence of benefit and probable harm from reduced cycling. Hagel et al.
provide no evidence to contradict this. Most people would consider it more
sensational to argue, as Hagel et al. do, that the helmet law should
extended to adults than, with no evidence of benefit and some suggestion
of harm, that it should be repealed.
References
1 Robinson DL. No clear evidence from countries that have enforced
the wearing of helmets. BMJ 2006;332:722-725.
Dear Editor and Authors,
Dr. Geary makes a very important point regarding the validity of the data on the use or non-use of bicycle helmet use abstracted from FARS and recently published by Cummings, et al, in their June, 2006 paper. This issue is one of the most important limitations and challenges in the use of narrative analysis. We previously struggled with a similar issue in a study published in Injury P...
Dear Editor,
The research letter from Vardy et al (1) seems to suggest that getting head teachers to lecture primary school kids about helmet-wearing made no difference to helmet-wearing rates among those attending hospital with cycling injuries. Neither did it alter the proportion of head injuries. But it was associated with a reduction in the total number of children attending hospital with cycling injuries...
Dear Editor,
In an editorial [1], you presented data from a study of peer reviews of 20 randomly selected papers submitted to Injury Prevention. Each paper was independently reviewed by three reviewers, who score...
Dear Editor,
Cummings et al. assume that bike helmets prevent 65% of deaths.[1] Yet a study of cyclist crashes in Brisbane concluded that helmets would prevent very few fatalities. All deaths were caused by bike/motor vehicle collisions. For 13 of the 14 non-helmeted cyclists who died, there was no indication that a helmet would have made any difference. The authors were very concerned about brain da...
Dear Editor,
It was not only fashionable but life saving for motorcyclists to use helmet while transiting in the 70s and 80s.Limited best practise based on knowledge couple with deteriorating standard of education and not only cost and warm climate may be the attributing factors resulting in the decline of helmet use in the present Nigeria.
Also, cultural or perhaps religious reasons may explain the use...
Dear Editor,
In response to our article, Burdett makes two main criticisms. The first relates to the issue of level of cycling activity in the community post-legislation. The second relates to our interpretation of the evidence for child cyclist helmet wearing when accompanying adults are helmeted compared with non-helmeted children. We consider these points separately.
On the issue of the level of cycl...
Dear Editor,
In their report on bicycle helmet use[1], Hagel et al recommend that Alberta's child helmet law be extended to include adults. They base this on (a) an increase in the rate of helmet use among the age group affected (under 18 years of age) from two years before the introduction of helmet legislation in 2002 to two years after, and (b) children being observed riding at higher rates of helmet use when accompa...
Dear Editor,
There is considerable debate about enforced helmet laws. Surveys in Australia counted several thousand cyclists before and after legislation, at the same sites, observation times and time of year. Percent helmet wearing (%HW) increased mainly because non-helmeted cyclists were discouraged from cycling – reductions in numbers counted were 2 to 15 times greater than the increases in numbers wearing helme...
McEvoy et al (2006) provide empirical evidence to support the case that distractions for the driver are an important feature of road crashes. There should be nothing too surprising in this; after all, many authorities recognise that an enforcible code of behaviour must be applied to public-service drivers; bus passengers are not likely to feel at ease with a driver whose attention deviates from the task in hand.
...
Dear Editor
According to good public policy, all laws with potentially detrimental effects (such as reduced cycling and reduced safety in numbers) should be evaluated. Far from being selective, my review examined every jurisdiction with a large increase in helmet wearing (more than 40 percentage points within a year). If helmet laws were beneficial, there should have been an obvious response. Yet there was no c...
Pages