Article Text

Download PDFPDF

Evidence for the ‘safety in density’ effect for cyclists: validation of agent-based modelling results
  1. Jason Hugh Thompson1,2,
  2. Jasper S Wijnands1,
  3. Suzanne Mavoa3,
  4. Katherine Scully1,
  5. Mark R Stevenson1
  1. 1 University of Melbourne, Transport, Health and Urban Design Research Hub, Parkville, Victoria, Australia
  2. 2 Centre for Human Factors and Sociotechnical Systems, University of the Sunshine Coast, Sippy Downs, Queensland, Australia
  3. 3 School of Population and Global Health, University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria, Australia
  1. Correspondence to Dr Jason Hugh Thompson, University of Melbourne, Transport, Health and Urban Design Research Hub, Parkville, VIC 3010, Australia; jason.thompson{at}unimelb.edu.au

Abstract

The safety in numbers (SiN) effect for cyclists is widely observed but remains poorly understood. Although most studies investigating the SiN phenomenon have focused on behavioural adaptation to ‘numbers’ of cyclists in the road network, previous work in simulated environments has suggested SiN may instead be driven by increases in local cyclist spatial density, which prevents drivers from attempting to move through groups of oncoming cyclists. This study therefore set out to validate the results of prior simulation studies in a real-world environment. Time-gap analysis of cyclists passing through an intersection was conducted using 5 hours of video observation of a single intersection in the city of Melbourne, Australia, where motorists were required to ‘yield’ to oncoming cyclists. Results demonstrated that potential collisions between motor vehicles and cyclists reduced with increasing cyclists per minute passing through the intersection. These results successfully validate those observed under simulated conditions, supporting evidence of a proposed causal mechanism related to safety in density rather than SiN, per se. Implications of these results for transportation planners, cyclists and transportation safety researchers are discussed, suggesting that increased cyclist safety could be achieved through directing cyclists towards focused, strategic corridors rather than dispersed across a network.

  • bicycle
  • mechanism, policy
  • interventions, public health
  • interventions, urban
  • populations
  • risk

Statistics from Altmetric.com

Request Permissions

If you wish to reuse any or all of this article please use the link below which will take you to the Copyright Clearance Center’s RightsLink service. You will be able to get a quick price and instant permission to reuse the content in many different ways.

Introduction

The safety in numbers (SiN) effect for cyclists is a widely referenced and observed but poorly understood phenomenon.1–3 Although a wide range of academic and applied studies cite SiN as a potential solution to car versus cyclist crashes (here referred to as ‘collisions’),3–7 there is currently little definitive evidence to guide policy-makers or transport planners in how to use SiN to create a safer cycling environment beyond simply encouraging ‘more cyclists’ into the system. Despite the lack of evidence surrounding the mechanism(s) that underpin it, the most widely forwarded understanding of SiN is that it is driven by ‘behavioural adaptation’ among drivers.8 9 That is, the more drivers see and become aware of cyclists on the road (or become cyclists, themselves), the more they learn to drive safely around them. Other authors have suggested that variation in risk associated with behavioural adaptation may be supplemented or offset by improvements to cycling infrastructure,10 seasonal characteristics,6 general traffic characteristics11 12 or behaviour of cyclists13 that coincide with changes in cycling rates.

There is little doubt that the SiN effect is genuine. Observed at both a macro-level and meso-level, there is consistency across studies that the per-capita number of collisions between cyclists and cars (ie, collision risk) for cyclists increases more slowly as more cyclists enter the system.3 However, beyond manifold studies that have used historic datasets of collisions and cyclist volumes to demonstrate this effect, as well as more recent work involving video recording of intersections across seasons,6 there is little research studying car versus cyclist interactions at the individual (micro) level. This has restricted the ability of studies to draw conclusions regarding the mechanisms underlying SiN that extend much beyond conjecture. It is important to address this knowledge gap as proposed interventions designed to reduce deaths and injuries among on-road cyclists may prove ineffective, or potentially more dangerous, without accurate understanding.

Recognising these limitations, previous work has attempted to create agent-based models (ABMs) of cyclist versus car interactions within simulated transport systems.14–16 ABMs allow both macro-level and micro-level behaviour of systems to be observed. Within such models, the behaviour of individual drivers and cyclists can be monitored alongside the overall performance (ie, safety) of the system.

The results of ABMs of car versus cyclist interactions have suggested that the SiN effect can be replicated in simulated transport systems where drivers demonstrate behavioural adaptation in response to exposure to cyclists.15 16 However, it can also be replicated in circumstances where drivers show no capacity to ‘learn’ to drive safely around cyclists at all.14 Rather, the SiN effect can be reproduced simply through encouraging behaviour that leads to the formation of higher-density cyclist groups. This reduces the surface area: volume ratio of cyclists to cars in a manner analogous to the ‘selfish-herd’ mechanism common to biological predator–prey relationships.17 18 The previous computational observations and synthetic evidence19 produced by these ABMs do not discount the fact that behavioural adaptation may be an additional mechanism contributing to the SiN effect, but suggests that behavioural adaptation is not a necessary condition for reducing risk given certain spatial configurations of cyclists in a network. Therefore, rather than SiN, the theory informed by agent-based modelling proposes that higher levels of safety can be achieved through increasing density; that is, safety in density (SiD).

While acknowledged as a potential mechanism,9 the SiD hypothesis has been criticised from the perspective that it is derived from computational models only, and that no in situ empirical evidence exists that cyclists ‘cluster’ in the real world. While the same concerns regarding lack of empirical evidence can also be applied to the behavioural adaptation hypothesis, it is somewhat simpler to address concerns related to spatial phenomena, which can be directly observed.

The focus of this study was therefore to observe micro-level interactions of cyclists and vehicles at an intersection mirroring that created in prior ABMs to determine how cyclist density is associated with per-capita collision risk for cyclists. In doing so, we aimed to gather empirical evidence of the SiD hypothesis’ operation in a real-world situation under conditions that prevented opportunity for behavioural adaptation. Consistent with previous macro-level research demonstrating a decreasing risk of collision per cyclist with increasing cyclist volume, it is hypothesised that increasing density of cyclists at a single intersection will be associated with a decreasing per-capita cyclist risk of potential collision with cars when interacting in ‘give-way’ or ‘yield’ situations.

Method

Intersection characteristics

Data collection occurred through video recording of naturalistic traffic behaviour occurring at an inner-city cross-intersection in Melbourne, Australia. The intersection was chosen for four reasons. First, it was located along a popular cycling route that featured a marked (not separated) cycling lane carrying a high volume of commuter cyclists to and from Melbourne’s central business district. This ensured that adequate volume of cyclist and car interactions could be logged and analysed within the recording period. Second, because the north–south route on which cyclists travelled was largely blocked to car traffic, the primary movement of cars at the intersection at the recording time was across the intersection in an east–west direction, producing consistent required judgements and behaviours among drivers. Third, because the selected intersection has previously recorded several car versus cyclist collisions, it has been identified as ‘high risk’ by local road authorities to the extent that it features high-visibility electronic, flashing signs that warn motorists that cyclists are present (see figure 1). In this respect, all drivers approaching the intersection were provided with standardised information designed to raise awareness that cyclists were potentially present. Lastly, the combination of intersection and vehicle characteristics described above was like those observed in the previously analysed simulated environments (ie, Thompson et al 14–16).

Figure 1

Selected intersection (pictured) was chosen based on high cyclist volume, consistency of car movements and standardised information regarding the possible presence of cyclists provided through warning signs.

Video recording of car and cyclist interactions

Video recording occurred over a period of five separate days over an 8-day period during daylight hours between 08:00 and 09:00 during early June 2017 to capture morning commuter traffic. Both a variety of days of the week and weather were sampled including 2× Mondays, one Wednesday, a Thursday and a Friday. One day of the week (Tuesday) was not captured due to heavy rain, making recording impractical. A small, concealed video camera was mounted in a tree on the median strip facing north-east at an angle of approximately 45 degrees to the centre of the intersection. This provided full view of oncoming northern and eastern traffic as well as cross-traffic from the west. A view of the selected intersection and indicative camera angle is shown in figures 2 and 3. Though the camera was concealed so as not to distract drivers or cyclists, researchers remained close to the intersection to answer questions from pedestrians or residents who may have noticed the researchers mounting and un-mounting the cameras at the beginning and end of each recording session.

Figure 2

View of the selected intersection showing orientation, location and coverage of the video camera.

Figure 3

View from the concealed camera of the intersection including a cyclist approaching from the north and two waiting drivers from the east.

Data coding

Time-stamped video footage was manually coded to an accuracy of 0.1 s by the research team (including review of coding accuracy) for all cyclist movements flowing north–south and car movements flowing east–west (see figure 3). All other car, pedestrian and bicycle movements were ignored for the purposes of this study. Instances where other vehicle or pedestrian movement potentially interfered with bicycle–vehicle interactions (eg, a north–south travelling car entered the intersection or pedestrians crossing the intersection) were also removed from analysis. The following data were coded for each cyclist as they moved through the intersection:

  • Cyclist unique identification number (identified per event, not by person);

  • Time at which the cyclist moved through the centre of the intersection; and

  • Time gap between the leading cyclist to the following cyclist.

Similarly, the following data were recorded for each driver movement:

  • Driver unique identification number (unrelated to vehicle license plate);

  • Time at which the driver arrived at the intersection and was at the front of the queue; and

  • Time at which the driver started crossing the intersection.

Recording these variables produced a temporally ordered list of cyclists and drivers that moved through the intersection over five 1-hour periods. This allowed calculation of variables, including:

  • The time gap of approaching cyclists to the intersection at the instant when drivers arrived at the intersection (s); number of cyclists each driver gave way to before moving away from the intersection (n);

  • Time gap between each cyclist passing through the intersection (s); and

  • Frequency of cyclists passing through the intersection during each recorded minute (cyclists/min).

Potential collisions were recorded as instances in which a gap of <4.2 s existed between a driver moving through the intersection (gap acceptance) and a cyclist then passing through. Potential collisions therefore represented instances in which drivers made a judgement to cross in front of a cyclist that less than 50% of all observed drivers made, leading them to potentially misjudge the distance or time in which it would take them to clear the intersection before a cyclist arrived (incorrect gap acceptance).20 The selection of the <4.2 s threshold for categorisation of potential collisions was made after analysis of gap acceptance and rejection distributions detailed in the Results section, below. Though 50% of drivers rejected gaps below 4.2 s (ie, waited), these instances were considered of negligible risk21 or ‘missed opportunities’20 by the driver.

Results

Figure 4 shows two kernel density estimates22 of the gap acceptance of drivers who reject (green) or accept the time gap (yellow) on arrival at the intersection. The distributions overlap, with gaps below 10 s generally those when drivers became more likely to ‘reject’ the time gap as too small for entry (observed: 0.1, 12.4) and longer gaps (>10 s) when drivers accepted the gap and moved through the intersection (observed: 2.3, 45.7).

Figure 4

Time-gap acceptance (yellow) or rejection (green) for drivers arriving at the intersection and arrival of approaching cyclists.

Of all observed drivers, 489 rejected the gap (waited), while 431 cars accepted the gap (did not wait). If drivers waited for cyclists at all, the mean number of bicycles they gave way to was 2.31 (SD 2.11). The probability that a driver accepted or rejected the time gap until the next cyclist reached the intersection is shown in figure 5. This figure shows a critical time gap23 of approximately 4.2 s at which half of the drivers decided to wait, while the other half did not wait. At this interval, cyclists experienced the most uncertain driver behaviour. The observed critical time gap was in the mid-range of previously modelled acceptance ranges of between 2 and 6 s.21 Note that the probability of a driver waiting did not monotonically decrease for increasing gap times, potentially due to the limited observational data in this research.

Figure 5

Probability estimates for driver behaviour based on the time gap to the next cyclist showing proportions of time-gap acceptance (yellow) and rejection (green).

Collision risk with variation in cyclist density

The frequency of potential collisions per minute (y) (accepted time gaps of <4.2 s) was modelled using the number of cyclists passing through the intersections per minute (x) as a single explanatory variable. Potential collisions and the number of cyclists were counted each minute across the total 5 hours of captured video footage, resulting in an appropriate sample of cyclist density and potential collision counts (n=385). Count data were not affected by overdispersion (p=0.99).24 Potential collisions between cars and cyclists were modelled using non-linear regression following Equation (1).

Embedded Image (1)

The expected number of collisions (y) with increasing numbers of cyclists per minute was therefore:

Embedded Image (2)

Evident from figure 6 is that potential crash numbers did not increase linearly with increasing density of cyclists per minute passing through the intersection. This effect of cyclist density on reducing potential collision risk per cyclist was independent of behavioural adaptation by drivers.

Figure 6

Estimated potential per-cyclist crashes (y-axis) with increasing count of cyclists per minute passing through the intersection (x-axis).

Discussion

The current study supported our hypothesis, demonstrating that potential per-cyclist collision risk did not increase at a rate that matched increases in cyclist density (ie, cyclist count per minute). It demonstrates the SiD hypothesis’ operation in a real-world situation under conditions where observations occurred within a limited period, precluding opportunity for behavioural adaptation by drivers. Extrapolated across a transport network, these findings are consistent with previous macro-level observations relating to the SiN effect. However, they make an important advance by providing further insight into a potential mechanism underlying the reduction in per-cyclist risk, namely, cyclist density.

In a series of ABMs, Thompson et al 14–16 demonstrated that the SiN effect could be reproduced within a synthetic road network with or without the influence of modelled behavioural adaptation among simulated drivers. This indicated that SiN may be better expressed as SiD because changes in the observed per-capita risk among simulated cyclists was driven by a spatial phenomenon primarily related to the density of cyclists in the system rather than numbers, per se. However, in a subsequent review of potential mechanisms underlying SiN, Jacobsen et al discounted the likelihood of this candidate mechanism, arguing that no empirical evidence of SiD had been demonstrated outside computational models.9 This study now provides such evidence, shedding potentially new light on mechanisms underpinning an issue that has interested transportation safety researchers for some time (eg, 1 2 12 25). For example, in a recent exploration and meta-analysis, Elvik12 questioned the causal mechanisms underlying SiN by highlighting an apparently counterintuitive relationship between cyclist volume and the strength of the safety in numbers effect. Elvik noted that it was weakest when cycling volumes were highest and that no clear relationship existed between the strength of the SiN effect and the ratio of motor vehicles to cyclists across a network. Viewed as a phenomenon that is the result of an aggregation of local level interactions, however, we consider that Elvik’s findings are consistent with results presented here.

That is, at specific times and locations where cyclist density is high, the movement of motor vehicles (and potential collisions) is restricted because time gaps between cyclists are smaller and more likely to be rejected by drivers. At specific locations and times when cyclist density is low, time gaps are larger and more likely to be accepted by drivers, resulting in higher per-capita collision risk as drivers and cyclists more frequently interact. These interactions considered at a micro-level may be independent of total cyclist numbers, total motor vehicle numbers, or motor vehicle:cyclist ratios when measured across the network.14 16 When total cyclists, motor vehicles and collisions are then measured at the level of a region, city or country, cyclist numbers versus reduced crash risk profiles may well emerge. However, it does not necessarily follow that cyclist numbers or ratios are the causal mechanism underlying SiN. Rather, higher cyclist numbers may simply make it more likely that locations of dense cycling activity occur within a network, which restricts unsafe interactions between cars and cyclists in those locations and results in reduced overall crash risk per cyclist.

Consistent with reduction in strength of the SiN effect at high cyclist or pedestrian volumes as reported by Elvik,12 the SiD effect may also be subject to saturation at very high volumes when gap rejection tolerances for individual drivers21 are exceeded. This can be seen in figure 6, where increases in estimated crashes slows risk slows considerably under conditions where a great number of cyclists pass through the intersection per minute (eg, >15). Conversely, if cyclist numbers are dispersed widely across a network rather than concentrated on particular roads or within selected areas, time gaps may be wider, more likely to be accepted by drivers, and the opportunity for the SiD mechanism to operate may be reduced.14 Again, this is not dissimilar to the selfish-herd mechanism observed in biological systems, which protects individual herd members from predators by reducing the surface area:volume ratio of the group.17 18 Dense groups of cyclists have a smaller surface area:volume ratio than dispersed groups, which provides a potentially protective effect against motor-vehicle incursions through the group.

The policy implications of these findings are important to consider for transportation planners, cyclists and researchers alike. For planners, they suggest that reducing risk of collision for cyclists in ‘yield’ situations can be achieved through increasing the density of cyclists along strategic routes. This could be achieved through identifying a well-connected network of cycling routes that are highlighted and recommended to cyclists. Cyclists themselves can be encouraged to use these routes in the knowledge that higher density cycling that reduces the time gap between themselves and other cyclists (to less than 4.2 s in the case of the observed intersection) may reduce car versus cyclist crash risk for themselves and fellow travellers. This is a departure from previous policy advice that has advocated that decreased crash risk can be achieved by increasing the number of cyclists across the network without consideration of cyclists’ immediate local context (eg, 5 26).

For researchers, these results highlight the use of ABMs to identify new theoretical models in silico that can then be tested in the field. To date, most work attempting to study SiN has been based on cross-sectional or longitudinal observations of crash numbers alongside cyclist and motor-vehicle volumes.3 11 The availability of aggregate data in this form originally drove the development of the SiN theory.4 However, it is possible that the strength of the original findings may have led to a subsequent degree of observer dependence, where data collection has largely continued to follow existing theory focused on ‘numbers’ of cyclists across a system rather than alternative spatial mechanisms as described here. Instead, the synthetic evidence collected from ABMs14–16 has enabled a new perspective on the way we could collect observational data27 related to cycling safety that has been operationalised in this study.

There are limitations to the present research that restrict generalisation of results. First, the studied interactions between drivers and cyclists were of purposefully designed, simple, select nature where only ‘give-way’ (‘yield’) behaviour by drivers was observed. Reductions in potential collision risk in situations where cyclists travelled in parallel with (ie, alongside) drivers were not studied. Further, though the density measure used in this study relates to time gaps between cyclists and between cyclist and car movements, the actual distance in metres was not recorded. It is therefore unclear whether drivers’ judgement on whether to accept or reject gaps was based on an estimate of time (eg, estimation of distance×speed)20 or simply distance from the cyclist, although it seems reasonable to assume that drivers incorporate the estimated speed of the cyclist (ie, time-based reasoning).

Another limitation is that the cyclist/motor vehicle interaction under study related to driver judgement and behaviour of west-travelling motor vehicles only. However, during observation, five near-misses and one minor crash were also witnessed for drivers travelling east, who failed to stop for oncoming cyclists at all. Each of these near-miss incidents required cyclists to take evasive action (ie, to brake heavily) and occurred in circumstances where drivers appeared to be blinded to on-coming cyclists by bright, early-morning sunshine (see figure 7). The SiD model as described here is unable to account for these ‘failed to see’28 occurrences where saliency of cyclists was either low or the driver’s perception was poor. Previous theoretical models have, however, attempted to account for these factors in the context of the SiD effect,16 providing an avenue for future observational studies to explore this issue. This highlights a final important limitation in that our methodology did not measure counts of actual collisions, but situations in which a collision was made more possible through a potentially risky driver manoeuvre. It therefore assumes that the ratio of actual collisions to potential collisions as defined here is robust.

Figure 7

Photograph taken from the intersection facing east, showing similar circumstances under which five near-misses and one minor collision were observed, possibly due to drivers being ‘blinded’ to the presence of cyclists by early-morning sunshine.

Conclusions

The current study has provided empirical validation of the SiD hypothesis’ operation in a real-world situation. It has demonstrated that the number of potential car versus cyclist collisions increases in a non-linear manner at a rate less than the increase in cyclists per minute. This reduction in risk per cyclist is associated with reduced time gaps between cyclists passing through intersections that prevent motor vehicles from moving between on-coming cyclists (gap rejection). Using a methodology based on synthetic evidence gathered from previous computational observation and experimentation,15 16 19 29 this work has provided further support for a candidate causal mechanism underlying the widely observed general relationship between cycling numbers and safety, a mechanism that has thus far eluded comprehensive explanation in the cycling safety literature.

What is already known on the subject

  • It is understood that greater numbers of cyclists in a road transport system reduces risk per cyclist, producing a ‘safety in numbers’ effect.

  • Potential mechanisms underlying the safety in numbers effect are contested.

  • A candidate causal mechanism underlying the safety in numbers effect has been suggested based on local spatial density of cyclists; that is, safety in density.

What this study adds

  • This work provides empirical validation of prior simulation results conducted using agent-based modelling.

  • Higher-density groups of passing cyclists prevent potential collisions between motor vehicles who reject available time gaps.

  • This mechanism provides insight into the ‘safety in numbers’ phenomenon, which has so far eluded comprehensive explanation.

References

  1. 1.
  2. 2.
  3. 3.
  4. 4.
  5. 5.
  6. 6.
  7. 7.
  8. 8.
  9. 9.
  10. 10.
  11. 11.
  12. 12.
  13. 13.
  14. 14.
  15. 15.
  16. 16.
  17. 17.
  18. 18.
  19. 19.
  20. 20.
  21. 21.
  22. 22.
  23. 23.
  24. 24.
  25. 25.
  26. 26.
  27. 27.
  28. 28.
  29. 29.

Footnotes

  • Contributors JHT conceived the research study collected the data and contributed to the analyses and manuscript writing. JSW contributed to the study design, conducted the analyses and contribute to the manuscript writing. SM contributed to the study design and manuscript writing. KS contributed to the data collection, analyses and manuscript writing. MRS contributed to the manuscript writing.

  • Funding This study was funded by the Australian Research Council (grant no. DE180101411) and the National Health and Medical Research Council (grant nos. 1043091 and 1121035).

  • Competing interests None declared.

  • Patient consent Not required.

  • Ethics approval The research was approved by the University of Melbourne Human Research Ethics Committee (1749212.1).

  • Provenance and peer review Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.

  • Data sharing statement The video recordings of the intersection interactions can be made available for researchers wishing to use them for additional studies.